The association between grandmaternal investment and early years overweight in the UK
9 pages
English

The association between grandmaternal investment and early years overweight in the UK

Le téléchargement nécessite un accès à la bibliothèque YouScribe
Tout savoir sur nos offres
9 pages
English
Le téléchargement nécessite un accès à la bibliothèque YouScribe
Tout savoir sur nos offres

Description

From the book : Evolutionary Psychology 11 issue 2 : 417-425.
Previous studies show that in many pre-modern and traditional populations the presence of a grandmother correlates with increased child survival rates, maybe as a result of improved child nutrition.
Grandmaternal investment aimed at improving grandchildren’s nutritional status in subsistence societies may have different outcomes in contemporary affluent societies.
Using the British Millennium Cohort Study I investigate the association between maternal and paternal grandmothers’ childcare and early years overweight in the UK.
Results show that children who were cared for mainly by their grandmothers between the ages of 9 months and 3 years were more likely overweight at age 3 than children who were cared for by their parents.
My results are in line with Pearce, Abbas, Ferguson, Graham, and Law (2010), although they did not distinguish grandmothers by lineage.
Grandmothers may influence children’s nutritional status in contemporary societies, but as with many evolved behavioral strategies the outcome may be no longer beneficial.

Sujets

Informations

Publié par
Publié le 01 janvier 2013
Nombre de lectures 39
Licence : En savoir +
Paternité, pas d'utilisation commerciale, partage des conditions initiales à l'identique
Langue English

Extrait

Evolutionary Psychology
www.epjournal.net – 2013. 11(2): 417425
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯
Original Article
The Association between Grandmaternal Investment and Early Overweight in the UK
Years
Antti O. Tanskanen, Department of Social Research, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. Email: antti.tanskanen@helsinki.fi(Corresponding author).
Abstract: studies show that in many premodern and traditional populations the Previous presence of a grandmother correlates with increased child survival rates, maybe as a result of improved child nutrition. Grandmaternal investment aimed at improving grandchildren’s nutritional status in subsistence societies may have different outcomes in contemporary affluent societies. Using the British Millennium Cohort Study I investigate the association between maternal and paternal grandmothers’ childcare and early years overweight in the UK. Results show that children who were cared for mainly by their grandmothers between the ages of 9 months and 3 years were more likely overweight at age 3 than children who were cared for by their parents. My results are in line with Pearce, Abbas, Ferguson, Graham, and Law (2010), although they did not distinguish grandmothers by lineage. Grandmothers may influence children’s nutritional status in contemporary societies, but as with many evolved behavioral strategies the outcome may be no longer beneficial.
Keywords:  childcare,grandparental investment, lineage, Millennium Cohort Study, obesity, overweight
¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯¯Introduction
Evolutionary theory argues that humans are a cooperative breeding species, which means that people other than the biological mother of a child can improve the child’s development, health, and wellbeing (Hrdy, 1999, 2009). Such alloparents include the father, aunts, uncles, and grandparents who may increase their inclusive fitness by investing in their close kin (Hamilton, 1964). In premodern and traditional populations the beneficial effects of kin investment are often measured by child survival (Coall and Hertwig, 2010). According to Sear and Mace’s (2008) review, grandmothers in particular have a positive effect on increased child survival. The Grandmother hypothesis states that the long postmenopausal life span of women might be an evolutionary adaptation, because of the support that these older women provide to their children and grandchildren (Lahdenperä, Lummaa, Helle, Tremblay, and
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
Russell, 2004). Women survive well beyond their fertile years, which is rare among animals, including other great apes (Hawkes, O’Connell, and Blurton Jones, 1997). The debate continues as to whether female menopause is indeed an adaptation or byproduct or something else (see e.g., Coall and Hertwig, 2010; Kachel, Premo, and Hublin, 2011; Lahdenperä, Gillespie, Lummaa, and Russell, 2012). Even though postmenopausal lifespan is not necessarily an adaptation, there is evidence for the beneficial impact of grandmothers’ presence to child survival. A significant way that grandmothers may have increased their grandchildren’s survival rates in premodern and traditional populations was to improve grandchildren’s nutritional status (Mace and Sear, 2005). For example, Hawkes and colleagues (1997) found that among Tanzanian huntergatherers, help especially from grandmothers improved children’s nutritional status (see also Sear, Mace, and McGregor, 2000). In subsistence societies, the grandmaternal support that improved children’s nutrition status may have significantly decreased child mortality. In modern western societies with low child mortality rates, grandmaternal support may still improve children’s nutritional status. However, grandmaternal support in modern societies may detrimentally affect children because it may increase the likelihood of a child becoming overweight. Studies have shown that childhood overweight and obesity correlates, for example, with increased risk of diabetes, heart diseases, and premature mortality in later life (see Al Mamun, Cramb, O’Callaghan, Williams, and Najman, 2009; Ebbeling, Pawlak, and Ludwig, 2002; Reilly and Kelly, 2011). Studies from contemporary societies show that living with grandparents may increase children’s risk for overweight (Davis, McGonagle, Schoeni, and Stafford, 2008; Velez et al., 2008; Watanabe, Lee, and Kawakubo, 2011). Pearce and colleagues (2010) studied with the British Millennium Cohort Study (the same data I use in this article) an association between childcare and child overweight. They found that children who were cared for by their grandparents were more likely to be overweight than those cared for by parents. The present study is an extension of the analysis of Pearce and colleagues (2010). My contribution is to distinguish the effects of maternal and paternal grandmothers. Many evolutionary studies have shown that maternal and paternal grandmothers are not in an equal position with each other both in premodern (e.g., Jamison, Cornell, Jamison, and Nakazato, 2002; Ragsdale, 2004; Voland and Beise, 2002) and modern (e.g., Bishop, Meyer, Schmidt, and Gray, 2009; Danielsbacka and Tanskanen, 2012; Danielsbacka, Tanskanen, Jokela, and Rotkirch, 2011; Euler and Weitzel, 1996; Pollet, Nettle, and Nelissen, 2006, 2007; Tanskanen, Rotkirch, and Danielsbacka, 2011) populations. These results are often explained with paternity uncertainty and the different reproductive interests between maternal and paternal grandparents (see Coall and Hertwig, 2010; Euler, 2011). Maternal and paternal grandmothers may not increase grandchildren’s nutritional status equally (Sear and Mace, 2008). Using data from rural Gambia, Sear and colleagues (2000) showed that grandchildren living with their nonreproductive maternal grandmothers had a greater likelihood of better nutritional status than others, but living with paternal grandmothers had no significant effect on grandchildren’s overall nutritional status. In addition, in the Kipsigis population from Kenya, kin support was associated with
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 418
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
infants’ wellbeing to a greater extent in rich than in poor families (Borgerhoff Mulder, 2007). In the present study, I analyze the association between grandmaternal childcare and child overweight in the UK. I am especially interested in whether maternal and paternal grandmothers have different effects. In addition, following Borgerhoff Mulder’s (2007) finding that in a developing country kin influences child wellbeing more in rich than in poor families, I study whether grandmaternal childcare has different effects according to mothers’ socioeconomic positions.
Materials and Methods
This study uses the Millennium Cohort Study (MCS) from a representative survey carried out in the UK. The aim of the MCS was to collect longitudinal information on children born at the beginning of the new millennium. The MCS looked at children, and their parents or parental figures who answered questions concerning their cohort member children. I analyzed the data from the second wave of the MCS. The second wave was collected in 20032005 when the children were approximately three years old (M = 37.7 months,SD2.5), and included 15,109 families (see Hansen, 2010 for more detailed data= description).Cases where the main respondents were the biological mothers of the child were selected. Only those cases where mothers lived with the cohort member child and child’s biological father were included. In cases of twins and triplets, only one child of the set was included. In addition, mothers who did not report their childcare arrangement and children with no height and weight information were excluded. These selections left me with 9,000 observations in the sample. The dependent variable was the child’s overweight (including obesity) at 3 years old. In the second wave of data collection, trained interviewers measured and weighed the cohort member children. Overweight was defined by the International Obesity Task Force cutoffs for body mass index, which are sex and age specific (at age 3 years: males = 17.89; females = 17.59) (Cole, Bellizzi, Flegal, and Dietz, 2000). Altogether, 23.6% of the children in the study sample were overweight. The main explanatory variable was the childcare arrangement reported by mothers. In the second wave of the MCS mothers reported whether they have used childcare arrangements and what the main arrangement was. Mothers were asked to report the main source of their childcare arrangement between the children’s ages of 9 months and 3 years. I have classified the scale into 6 categories: 1 = Parents (no care, respondent herself or partner), 2 = Maternal grandmother, 3 = Paternal grandmother, 4 = Other relative, 5 = Friend or neighbor, 6 = Formal provider (registered childminder, unregistered childminder, workplace or college day nursery crèche, local authority day nursery crèche or private day nursery crèche) (see Table 1).
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 419
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
Parent  Maternal grandmother  Paternal grandmother  Other relative  Friend or neighbor  Formal Birthweight(meankg)
Ethnicity (%) White  Mixed  Indian  Pakistani and Bangladeshi  Black  Other Semiroutine, routine and not working (SEC 1)  Lower supervisory and technical (SEC 2)  Small employers and own account (SEC 3)  Intermediate (SEC 4) Mother smoked during pregnancy (%) No
5,601 1,156 477 259 91 1,416 9,000
7,886 195 233 421 147 118 3,168482 377 1,767
6,303
62.2 12.8 5.3 2.9 1.0 15.7 3.4
87.6 2.2 2.6 4.7 1.6 1.3
35.2 5.4 4.2 19.6
70.0
Normal weight 6,323 70.3 Country (%) England 5,604 62.3  Wales 1,320 14.7  Scotland 1,163 12.9 I ran my analyses in two phases. First, I employed binary logistic regression models to analyze an association between childcare type and child’s overweight. Second, I studied an interaction between grandmaternal care and mothers’ socioeconomic circumstances. I ran the models as unadjusted and adjusted. Since the results in adjusted and unadjusted models are quite similar I show only the adjusted results. In the adjusted models I Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 420
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
controlled for several potential confounding variables that are shown to correlate with early years overweight (see Brophy et al., 2009; Pearce et al., 2010). These factors are child’s birth weight, number of siblings in household, ethnicity, mothers’ socioeconomic circumstance, smoking during pregnancy, prepregnancy weight status, and country (see Table 1). Since mother's age, breastfeeding duration, education, and the existence of maternal or paternal grandmothers do not significantly correlate with child’s overweight status these variables were not added in the models. With the exception of the child’s birth weight and number of siblings, all variables are categorical. For the analyses I have transformed them into the dummy variables.
Results
Table2showstheresults(theoverallmodel:2LL=9485.6;χ²= 340.7,df = 21, p< 0.0001; Nagelkerke= 0.056). Parent was chosen as the reference category; an odds ratio above 1 indicates higher likelihood for child’s overweight compared to those who receive main child care from their parents, and an odds ratio below 1 indicates the opposite. Child care support from relatives other than grandmothers or friends and neighbors between the child’s ages 9 months and 3 years increase the likelihood for child’s overweight at the age of 3. However, these differences were not statistically significant. In addition, formal childcare arrangements did not increase or decrease the likelihood for early years overweight. The results are quite similar in unadjusted and adjusted models. Table 2. child’s overweight (including obesity) at the age of 3 years by Predicting childcare type between the ages of 9 months and 3 years (odds ratios and 95% confidence
1.03 0.97
Maternal grandmother 1.20 0.09 2.34 .019 1.40 Paternal grandmother 1.21 0.14 1.67 .095 1.51 Other relative 1.09 0.16 0.58 .562 0.81 1.46 Friend or neighbor 0.95 0.25 0.22 .829 0.57 1.58 Formal 1.02 0.08 0.23 .818 0.87 1.19 Table 2 also shows that in the case of maternal and paternal grandmothers the odds ratios are above 1, which means that those children who receive primary childcare from their grandmothers are more likely overweight than those who receive childcare from parents (parent 22.9%, 95% CI = 21.824.0; MGM 26.1%, 95% CI = 23.628.6,p= .019; PGM 26.2%, 95% CI = 22.330.1,p= .095;n= 9,000). The difference is significant in the case of maternal, but not paternal, grandmothers. This is due to the relatively low number of paternal grandmothers who are the main childcare providers. When compared between maternal and paternal grandmothers results show no statistically significant differences:
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 421
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
paternal grandmothers being the reference category, maternal grandmothers’ odds ratio is 0.97 (95% CIs = 0.751.24,p= .795;n1,633) (results not shown in tables).= Next, I study the interaction between grandmaternal care and mothers’ socioeconomic circumstances. The results are presented in Table 3 (the overall model: 2LL = 7630.4;χ²= 277.1,df= 21,p< 0.0001; Nagelkerke= 0.057). Both grandmother types are included in the same category since the previous analyses show that they do not differ from each other. The results do not show significant differences according to mothers’ socioeconomic circumstances. Table 3.obesity) at the age of 3 years: InteractionPredicting child’s overweight (including effects between childcare type (between the ages of 9 months and 3 years) and maternal
Grandmaternal childcare (ref. = parent)
SEC 1 (ref.) SEC 2 SEC 3 SEC 4 SEC 5
Grandmaternal childcare × SEC 2 Grandmaternal childcare × SEC 3 Grandmaternal childcare × SEC 4
Discussion
1.31
1.000.88 0.81 0.84
0.94
0.98 0.70 0.91
0.16
0.12 0.13 0.08
0.08
0.29 0.26 0.17
2.24
0.93 1.28 1.82
0.70
0.06 0.95 0.50
.025
.353 .200 .068 .482
.952 .343 .614
95% CIs
1.03
0.67 0.58 0.70 0.80
0.55 0.33 0.64
1.66
1.16 1.12 1.01 1.11
1.77 1.47 1.31
The present article studied the association between grandmothers’ childcare support and grandchildren’s overweight in the UK. Children whose main child minder between the ages of 9 months and 3 years was a grandmother were more likely to be overweight at the age of 3 than those cared for by their parents. The results are in line with the study of Pearce and colleagues (2010) with the same data. Thus, the present study showed grandmothers’ investment correlating with grandchildren’s increased risk of overweight in contemporary societies, but this does not vary by lineage. That is to say, grandmothers may impact grandchildren’s nutritional status in modern states as they did in some traditional populations (see Hawkes et al., 1997; Sear et al., 2000). Findings from the present study show that maternal and paternal grandmothers seem to increase the risk of child overweight equally. According to Borgerhoff Mulder’s (2007) analysis, kin increase child wellbeing
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 422
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
more in rich than in poor families. In the present study, I analyzed interaction effects between grandmaternal childcare and mothers’ socioeconomic circumstances. However, I did not find convincing support for the prediction that grandmaternal care differently influences child overweight status in different socioeconomic positions. Previous studies have shown that in contemporary societies grandparents may improve grandchild wellbeing. Many studies have detected a correlation between grandparental investment and child wellbeing measured, for example, by the child’s psychological adjustment, academic achievement, and mental and physical development (e.g., Tanskanen and Danielsbacka, 2012; see Sear and Coall, 2011 for review). The present study shows, however, that grandparental investment may not always be beneficial for children in contemporary societies. To conclude, the results show that the same grandmaternal behavior that may have had beneficial effects for grandchildren in our evolutionary past may have opposite effects in modern societies.
Acknowledgements:I thank Mirkka Danielsbacka, Markus Jokela, Anna Rotkirch, two anonymous reviewers and the editor Rebecca Sear for their helpful comments. The study is part of the Generational Transmissions in Finland (Gentrans) project at the University of Helsinki funded by the Academy of Finland (250620) and Alli Paasikivi foundation.
Received 4 November 2012; Revision submitted 23 May 2013; Accepted 28 May 2013
References
Al Mamun, A., Cramb, S. M., O’Callaghan, M. J., Williams, G. M., and Najman, J. M. (2009). Childhood overweight status predicts diabetes at age 21 years: A followup study.Obesity, 17, 12551261. Bishop, D. B., Meyer, B. C., Schmidt, T. M., and Gray, B. R. (2009). Differential investment behavior between grandparents and grandchildren: The role of paternity uncertainty.Evolutionary Psychology,7, 6677. Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2007). Hamilton’s rule and kin competition: The Kipsigis case. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 299312. Brophy, S., Cooksey, R., Gravenor, M. B., Mistry, R., Thomas, N., Lyons, R. A., and Williams, R. (2009). Risk factors for childhood obesity at age 5: Analysis of the Millennium Cohort Study.BMC Public Health, 9, 467. Coall, D. A., and Hertwig, R. (2010). Grandparental investment: Past, present, and future. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,33, 159. Cole T. J., Bellizzi, M. C., Flegal, K. M., and Dietz, W. H. (2000). Establishing a standard definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide: International survey.BMJ, 3, 16. Danielsbacka, M., and Tanskanen, A. O. (2012). Adolescent grandchildren’s perceptions of grandparents’ involvement in UK: An interpretation from life course and evolutionary theory perspective.European Journal of Ageing,9, 329341. Danielsbacka, M., Tanskanen, A. O., Jokela, M., and Rotkirch, A. (2011). Grandparental child care in Europe: Evidence for preferential investment in more certain kin.
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 423
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
Evolutionary Psychology,9, 324. Davis, M. M., McGonagle, K., Schoeni, R. F., and Stafford, F. (2008). Grandparental and parental obesity influences on childhood overweight: implications for primary care practice.Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 21The , 549554. Ebbeling, C., Pawlak, D., and Ludwig, D. (2002). Childhood Obesity: Publichealth crisis, common sense cure.Lancet, 360, 473482. Euler, H. A. (2011). Grandparents and extended kin. In C. Salmon and T. K. Shackelford (Eds.),The oxford handbook on evolutionary family psychology(pp. 181210). New York: Oxford University Press. Euler, H. A., and Weitzel, B. (1996). Discriminative grandparental solicitude as reproductive strategy.Human Nature,7, 3959. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour I and II.Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 152. Hansen, K. (Ed.) (2010).Millennium Cohort Study first, second, third and fourth surveys: A guide to the datasets fifth edition.London: Centre for Longitudinal Studies. Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., and Blurton Jones, N. G. (1997). Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning and the evolution of long postmenopausal life spans.Current Anthropology,38, 551578. Hrdy, S. B. (1999).Mother nature: Maternal instincts and how they shape the human species.New York: Ballantine Books. Hrdy, S. B. (2009).Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding.Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. Jamison, C. S., Cornell, L. L., Jamison, P. L., and Nakazato, H. (2002). Are all grandmothers equal? A review and a preliminary test of the “grandmother hypothesis” in Tokugawa, Japan.American Journal of Physical Anthropology,119, 6776. Kachel, A. F., Premo, L. S., and Hublin, J.J. (2011). Grandmothering and natural selection. Proceeding of the Royal Society B: Biological Science, 278, 384391. Lahdenperä M., Gillespie D. O. S., Lummaa V., and Russell, A. F. (2012). Severe intergenerational reproductive conflict and the evolution of menopause.Ecology Letters, 15, 12831290. Lahdenperä, M., Lummaa, V., Helle, S., Tremblay, M., and Russell, A. F. (2004). Fitness benefits of prolonged postreproductive lifespan in women.Nature, 428, 178181. Mace, R., and Sear, R. (2005). Are humans cooperative breeders? In E. Voland, A. Chasiotis, and W. Schiefenhoevel (Eds.),Grandmotherhood: The evolutionary significance of the second half of female life(pp. 143159). Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Pearce, A., Li, L., Abbas, J., Ferguson, B., Graham, H., and Law, C. (2010). Is childcare associated with the risk of overweight and obesity in the early years? Findings from the UK Millennium Cohort Study.International Journal of Obesity, 34, 11601168. Pollet, T. V., Nettle, D., and Nelissen, M. (2006). Contact frequencies between grandparents and grandchildren in a modern society: Estimates of the impact of paternity uncertainty.Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology,4, 203213. Pollet, T. V., Nettle, D., and Nelissen, M. (2007). Maternal grandmothers do go the extra
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 424
Grandmaternal investment and early years overweight
mile: Factoring distance and lineage into differential contact with grandchildren. Evolutionary Psychology,5, 832843. Ragsdale, G. (2004). Grandmothering in Cambridgeshire, 17701861.Human Nature, 15, 301317. Reilly, J. J., and Kelly, J. (2011). Longterm impact of overweight and obesity in childhood and adolescence on morbidity and premature mortality in adulthood: Systematic review.International Journal of Obesity, 35, 891898. Sear, R., and Coall, D. A. (2011). How much does family matter? Cooperative breeding and the demographic transition.Population and Development Review,37, 81112. Sear, R., and Mace, R. (2008). Who keeps children alive? A review of the effects of kin on child survival.Evolution and Human Behavior,29, 118. Sear, R., Mace, R., and McGregor, I. A. (2000). Maternal grandmothers improve the nutritional status and survival of children in rural Gambia.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences,267, 461467. Tanskanen, A. O., and Danielsbacka, M. (2012). Beneficial effects of grandparental involvement vary by lineage in the UK.Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 985988. Tanskanen, A. O., Rotkirch, A., and Danielsbacka, M. (2011). Do grandparents favor granddaughters? Biased grandparental investment in UK.Evolution and Human Behavior,32, 407415. Velez, J. C., Fitzpatrick, A. L., Barbosa, C. I., Diaz, M., Urzua, M., and Andrade, A. H. (2008). Nutritional status and obesity in children and young adults with disabilities in Punta Arenas, Patagonia, Chile.International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 31, 305313. Voland, E., and Beise, J. (2002). Opposite effects of maternal and paternal grandmothers on infant survival in historical Krummhörn.Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 52, 435443. Watanabe, E., Lee, J. S., and Kawakubo, K. (2011). Associations of maternal employment and threegeneration families with preschool children’s overweight and obesity in Japan.International Journal of Obesity, 35, 945952.
Evolutionary Psychology – ISSN 14747049 – Volume 11(2). 2013. 425
  • Univers Univers
  • Ebooks Ebooks
  • Livres audio Livres audio
  • Presse Presse
  • Podcasts Podcasts
  • BD BD
  • Documents Documents